- Bipolar Disorder
- Race and Identity
- Stress Management
- Brain Health
- Relationships
- Online Therapy
- History and Biographies
- Student Resources
- Sleep and Dreaming
- Self-Improvement
- Mental Strength
- Family & Relationships
- Anxiety & Depression
- Coronavirus
- Mental Health
- Verywell Mind Insights
- The Winter Issue
- Editorial Process
- Meet Our Review Board
- Crisis Support

The Definition of Random Assignment According to Psychology
Kendra Cherry, MS, is an author and educational consultant focused on helping students learn about psychology.
:max_bytes(150000):strip_icc():format(webp)/IMG_9791-89504ab694d54b66bbd72cb84ffb860e.jpg)
Emily is a board-certified science editor who has worked with top digital publishing brands like Voices for Biodiversity, Study.com, GoodTherapy, Vox, and Verywell.
:max_bytes(150000):strip_icc():format(webp)/Emily-Swaim-1000-0f3197de18f74329aeffb690a177160c.jpg)
Materio / Getty Images
Random assignment refers to the use of chance procedures in psychology experiments to ensure that each participant has the same opportunity to be assigned to any given group. Study participants are randomly assigned to different groups, such as the experimental group or treatment group.
Random assignment might involve tactics such as flipping a coin, drawing names out of a hat, rolling dice, or assigning random numbers to participants.
It is important to note that random assignment differs from random selection . While random selection refers to how participants are randomly chosen to represent the larger population, random assignment refers to how those chosen participants are then assigned to experimental groups.
Random Assignment In Research
To determine if changes in one variable lead to changes in another variable, psychologists must perform an experiment. Researchers often begin by forming a testable hypothesis predicting that one variable of interest will have some impact on another variable.
The variable that the experimenters will manipulate in the experiment is known as the independent variable , while the variable that they will then measure is known as the dependent variable. While there are different ways to look at relationships between variables, an experiment is the best way to get a clear idea if there is a cause-and-effect relationship between two or more variables.
Once researchers have formulated a hypothesis, conducted background research, and chosen an experimental design, it is time to find participants for their experiment. How exactly do researchers decide who will be part of an experiment? As mentioned previously, this is often accomplished through something known as random selection.
Random Selection
In order to generalize the results of an experiment to a larger group, it is important to choose a sample that is representative of the qualities found in that population. For example, if the total population is 51% female and 49% male, then the sample should reflect those same percentages.
Choosing a representative sample is often accomplished by randomly picking people from the population to be participants in a study. Random selection means that everyone in the group stands an equal chance of being chosen. Once a pool of participants has been selected, it is time to assign them into groups.
By randomly assigning the participants into groups, the experimenters can be fairly sure that each group will be the same before the independent variable is applied.
Participants might be randomly assigned to the control group , which does not receive the treatment in question. Or they might be randomly assigned to the experimental group , which does receive the treatment.
Random assignment increases the likelihood that the two groups are the same at the outset. That way any changes that result from the application of the independent variable can be assumed to be the result of the treatment of interest.
Example of Random Assignment
Imagine that a researcher is interested in learning whether or not drinking caffeinated beverages prior to an exam will improve test performance. After randomly selecting a pool of participants, each person is randomly assigned to either the control group or the experimental group.
The participants in the control group consume a placebo drink prior to the exam that does not contain any caffeine. Those in the experimental group, on the other hand, consume a caffeinated beverage before taking the test.
Participants in both groups then take the test, and the researcher compares the results to determine if the caffeinated beverage had any impact on test performance.
A Word From Verywell
Random assignment plays an important role in the psychology research process. Not only does this process help eliminate possible sources of bias, but it also makes it easier to generalize the results of a tested sample population to a larger population.
Random assignment helps ensure that members of each group in the experiment are the same, which means that the groups are also likely more representative of what is present in the larger population. Through the use of this technique, psychology researchers are able to study complex phenomena and contribute to our understanding of the human mind and behavior.
Sullivan L. Random assignment versus random selection . In: The SAGE Glossary of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc.; 2009. doi:10.4135/9781412972024.n2108
Lin Y, Zhu M, Su Z. The pursuit of balance: An overview of covariate-adaptive randomization techniques in clinical trials . Contemp Clin Trials. 2015;45(Pt A):21-25. doi:10.1016/j.cct.2015.07.011
Alferes VR. Methods of Randomization in Experimental Design. Los Angeles: SAGE; 2012.
Nestor PG, Schutt RK. Research Methods in Psychology: Investigating Human Behavior. Los Angeles: SAGE; 2015.
By Kendra Cherry Kendra Cherry, MS, is an author and educational consultant focused on helping students learn about psychology.
By clicking “Accept All Cookies”, you agree to the storing of cookies on your device to enhance site navigation, analyze site usage, and assist in our marketing efforts.
Have a language expert improve your writing
Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, generate accurate citations for free.
- Knowledge Base
- Methodology
- Random Assignment in Experiments | Introduction & Examples
Random Assignment in Experiments | Introduction & Examples
Published on March 8, 2021 by Pritha Bhandari . Revised on February 13, 2023.
In experimental research, random assignment is a way of placing participants from your sample into different treatment groups using randomization.
With simple random assignment, every member of the sample has a known or equal chance of being placed in a control group or an experimental group. Studies that use simple random assignment are also called completely randomized designs .
Random assignment is a key part of experimental design . It helps you ensure that all groups are comparable at the start of a study: any differences between them are due to random factors, not research biases like sampling bias or selection bias .
Table of contents
Why does random assignment matter, random sampling vs random assignment, how do you use random assignment, when is random assignment not used, frequently asked questions about random assignment.
Random assignment is an important part of control in experimental research, because it helps strengthen the internal validity of an experiment and avoid biases.
In experiments, researchers manipulate an independent variable to assess its effect on a dependent variable, while controlling for other variables. To do so, they often use different levels of an independent variable for different groups of participants.
This is called a between-groups or independent measures design.
You use three groups of participants that are each given a different level of the independent variable:
- a control group that’s given a placebo (no dosage, to control for a placebo effect ),
- an experimental group that’s given a low dosage,
- a second experimental group that’s given a high dosage.
Random assignment to helps you make sure that the treatment groups don’t differ in systematic ways at the start of the experiment, as this can seriously affect (and even invalidate) your work.
If you don’t use random assignment, you may not be able to rule out alternative explanations for your results.
- participants recruited from cafes are placed in the control group ,
- participants recruited from local community centers are placed in the low dosage experimental group,
- participants recruited from gyms are placed in the high dosage group.
With this type of assignment, it’s hard to tell whether the participant characteristics are the same across all groups at the start of the study. Gym-users may tend to engage in more healthy behaviors than people who frequent cafes or community centers, and this would introduce a healthy user bias in your study.
Although random assignment helps even out baseline differences between groups, it doesn’t always make them completely equivalent. There may still be extraneous variables that differ between groups, and there will always be some group differences that arise from chance.
Most of the time, the random variation between groups is low, and, therefore, it’s acceptable for further analysis. This is especially true when you have a large sample. In general, you should always use random assignment in experiments when it is ethically possible and makes sense for your study topic.
Random sampling and random assignment are both important concepts in research, but it’s important to understand the difference between them.
Random sampling (also called probability sampling or random selection) is a way of selecting members of a population to be included in your study. In contrast, random assignment is a way of sorting the sample participants into control and experimental groups.
While random sampling is used in many types of studies, random assignment is only used in between-subjects experimental designs.
Some studies use both random sampling and random assignment, while others use only one or the other.

Random sampling enhances the external validity or generalizability of your results, because it helps ensure that your sample is unbiased and representative of the whole population. This allows you to make stronger statistical inferences .
You use a simple random sample to collect data. Because you have access to the whole population (all employees), you can assign all 8000 employees a number and use a random number generator to select 300 employees. These 300 employees are your full sample.
Random assignment enhances the internal validity of the study, because it ensures that there are no systematic differences between the participants in each group. This helps you conclude that the outcomes can be attributed to the independent variable .
- a control group that receives no intervention.
- an experimental group that has a remote team-building intervention every week for a month.
You use random assignment to place participants into the control or experimental group. To do so, you take your list of participants and assign each participant a number. Again, you use a random number generator to place each participant in one of the two groups.
Here's why students love Scribbr's proofreading services
Discover proofreading & editing
To use simple random assignment, you start by giving every member of the sample a unique number. Then, you can use computer programs or manual methods to randomly assign each participant to a group.
- Random number generator: Use a computer program to generate random numbers from the list for each group.
- Lottery method: Place all numbers individually in a hat or a bucket, and draw numbers at random for each group.
- Flip a coin: When you only have two groups, for each number on the list, flip a coin to decide if they’ll be in the control or the experimental group.
- Use a dice: When you have three groups, for each number on the list, roll a dice to decide which of the groups they will be in. For example, assume that rolling 1 or 2 lands them in a control group; 3 or 4 in an experimental group; and 5 or 6 in a second control or experimental group.
This type of random assignment is the most powerful method of placing participants in conditions, because each individual has an equal chance of being placed in any one of your treatment groups.
Random assignment in block designs
In more complicated experimental designs, random assignment is only used after participants are grouped into blocks based on some characteristic (e.g., test score or demographic variable). These groupings mean that you need a larger sample to achieve high statistical power .
For example, a randomized block design involves placing participants into blocks based on a shared characteristic (e.g., college students versus graduates), and then using random assignment within each block to assign participants to every treatment condition. This helps you assess whether the characteristic affects the outcomes of your treatment.
In an experimental matched design , you use blocking and then match up individual participants from each block based on specific characteristics. Within each matched pair or group, you randomly assign each participant to one of the conditions in the experiment and compare their outcomes.
Sometimes, it’s not relevant or ethical to use simple random assignment, so groups are assigned in a different way.
When comparing different groups
Sometimes, differences between participants are the main focus of a study, for example, when comparing men and women or people with and without health conditions. Participants are not randomly assigned to different groups, but instead assigned based on their characteristics.
In this type of study, the characteristic of interest (e.g., gender) is an independent variable, and the groups differ based on the different levels (e.g., men, women, etc.). All participants are tested the same way, and then their group-level outcomes are compared.
When it’s not ethically permissible
When studying unhealthy or dangerous behaviors, it’s not possible to use random assignment. For example, if you’re studying heavy drinkers and social drinkers, it’s unethical to randomly assign participants to one of the two groups and ask them to drink large amounts of alcohol for your experiment.
When you can’t assign participants to groups, you can also conduct a quasi-experimental study . In a quasi-experiment, you study the outcomes of pre-existing groups who receive treatments that you may not have any control over (e.g., heavy drinkers and social drinkers). These groups aren’t randomly assigned, but may be considered comparable when some other variables (e.g., age or socioeconomic status) are controlled for.
In experimental research, random assignment is a way of placing participants from your sample into different groups using randomization. With this method, every member of the sample has a known or equal chance of being placed in a control group or an experimental group.
Random selection, or random sampling , is a way of selecting members of a population for your study’s sample.
In contrast, random assignment is a way of sorting the sample into control and experimental groups.
Random sampling enhances the external validity or generalizability of your results, while random assignment improves the internal validity of your study.
Random assignment is used in experiments with a between-groups or independent measures design. In this research design, there’s usually a control group and one or more experimental groups. Random assignment helps ensure that the groups are comparable.
In general, you should always use random assignment in this type of experimental design when it is ethically possible and makes sense for your study topic.
To implement random assignment , assign a unique number to every member of your study’s sample .
Then, you can use a random number generator or a lottery method to randomly assign each number to a control or experimental group. You can also do so manually, by flipping a coin or rolling a dice to randomly assign participants to groups.
Cite this Scribbr article
If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the “Cite this Scribbr article” button to automatically add the citation to our free Citation Generator.
Bhandari, P. (2023, February 13). Random Assignment in Experiments | Introduction & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved February 27, 2023, from https://www.scribbr.com/methodology/random-assignment/
Is this article helpful?

Pritha Bhandari
Other students also liked, guide to experimental design | overview, steps, & examples, confounding variables | definition, examples & controls, control groups and treatment groups | uses & examples, what is your plagiarism score.
Random Sampling vs. Random Assignment
Random sampling and random assignment are fundamental concepts in the realm of research methods and statistics. However, many students struggle to differentiate between these two concepts, and very often use these terms interchangeably. Here we will explain the distinction between random sampling and random assignment.
Random sampling refers to the method you use to select individuals from the population to participate in your study. In other words, random sampling means that you are randomly selecting individuals from the population to participate in your study. This type of sampling is typically done to help ensure the representativeness of the sample (i.e., external validity). It is worth noting that a sample is only truly random if all individuals in the population have an equal probability of being selected to participate in the study. In practice, very few research studies use “true” random sampling because it is usually not feasible to ensure that all individuals in the population have an equal chance of being selected. For this reason, it is especially important to avoid using the term “random sample” if your study uses a nonprobability sampling method (such as convenience sampling).

Discover How We Assist to Edit Your Dissertation Chapters
Aligning theoretical framework, gathering articles, synthesizing gaps, articulating a clear methodology and data plan, and writing about the theoretical and practical implications of your research are part of our comprehensive dissertation editing services.
- Bring dissertation editing expertise to chapters 1-5 in timely manner.
- Track all changes, then work with you to bring about scholarly writing.
- Ongoing support to address committee feedback, reducing revisions.

Random assignment refers to the method you use to place participants into groups in an experimental study. For example, say you are conducting a study comparing the blood pressure of patients after taking aspirin or a placebo. You have two groups of patients to compare: patients who will take aspirin (the experimental group) and patients who will take the placebo (the control group). Ideally, you would want to randomly assign the participants to be in the experimental group or the control group, meaning that each participant has an equal probability of being placed in the experimental or control group. This helps ensure that there are no systematic differences between the groups before the treatment (e.g., the aspirin or placebo) is given to the participants. Random assignment is a fundamental part of a “true” experiment because it helps ensure that any differences found between the groups are attributable to the treatment, rather than a confounding variable.
So, to summarize, random sampling refers to how you select individuals from the population to participate in your study. Random assignment refers to how you place those participants into groups (such as experimental vs. control). Knowing this distinction will help you clearly and accurately describe the methods you use to collect your data and conduct your study.
Popular searches
- How to Get Participants For Your Study
- How to Do Segmentation?
- Conjoint Preference Share Simulator
- MaxDiff Analysis
- Likert Scales
- Reliability & Validity
Request consultation
Do you need support in running a pricing or product study? We can help you with agile consumer research and conjoint analysis.
Looking for an online survey platform?
Conjointly offers a great survey tool with multiple question types, randomisation blocks, and multilingual support. The Basic tier is always free.

Research Methods Knowledge Base
- Navigating the Knowledge Base
- Foundations
- Measurement
- Internal Validity
- Introduction to Design
- Types of Designs
- Probabilistic Equivalence
Random Selection & Assignment
- Defining Experimental Designs
- Factorial Designs
- Randomized Block Designs
- Covariance Designs
- Hybrid Experimental Designs
- Quasi-Experimental Design
- Pre-Post Design Relationships
- Designing Designs for Research
- Quasi-Experimentation Advances
- Table of Contents
Fully-functional online survey tool with various question types, logic, randomisation, and reporting for unlimited number of responses and surveys.
Completely free for academics and students .
Random selection is how you draw the sample of people for your study from a population. Random assignment is how you assign the sample that you draw to different groups or treatments in your study.
It is possible to have both random selection and assignment in a study. Let’s say you drew a random sample of 100 clients from a population list of 1000 current clients of your organization. That is random sampling. Now, let’s say you randomly assign 50 of these clients to get some new additional treatment and the other 50 to be controls. That’s random assignment.
It is also possible to have only one of these (random selection or random assignment) but not the other in a study. For instance, if you do not randomly draw the 100 cases from your list of 1000 but instead just take the first 100 on the list, you do not have random selection. But you could still randomly assign this nonrandom sample to treatment versus control. Or, you could randomly select 100 from your list of 1000 and then nonrandomly (haphazardly) assign them to treatment or control.
And, it’s possible to have neither random selection nor random assignment. In a typical nonequivalent groups design in education you might nonrandomly choose two 5th grade classes to be in your study. This is nonrandom selection. Then, you could arbitrarily assign one to get the new educational program and the other to be the control. This is nonrandom (or nonequivalent) assignment.
Random selection is related to sampling . Therefore it is most related to the external validity (or generalizability) of your results. After all, we would randomly sample so that our research participants better represent the larger group from which they’re drawn. Random assignment is most related to design . In fact, when we randomly assign participants to treatments we have, by definition, an experimental design . Therefore, random assignment is most related to internal validity . After all, we randomly assign in order to help assure that our treatment groups are similar to each other (i.e., equivalent) prior to the treatment.
Which one are you?
I am new to conjointly, i am already using conjointly.
Purpose and Limitations of Random Assignment
In an experimental study, random assignment is a process by which participants are assigned, with the same chance, to either a treatment or a control group. The goal is to assure an unbiased assignment of participants to treatment options.
Random assignment is considered the gold standard for achieving comparability across study groups, and therefore is the best method for inferring a causal relationship between a treatment (or intervention or risk factor) and an outcome.

Random assignment of participants produces comparable groups regarding the participants’ initial characteristics, thereby any difference detected in the end between the treatment and the control group will be due to the effect of the treatment alone.
How does random assignment produce comparable groups?
1. random assignment prevents selection bias.
Randomization works by removing the researcher’s and the participant’s influence on the treatment allocation. So the allocation can no longer be biased since it is done at random, i.e. in a non-predictable way.
This is in contrast with the real world, where for example, the sickest people are more likely to receive the treatment.
2. Random assignment prevents confounding
A confounding variable is one that is associated with both the intervention and the outcome, and thus can affect the outcome in 2 ways:

Either directly:

Or indirectly through the treatment:

This indirect relationship between the confounding variable and the outcome can cause the treatment to appear to have an influence on the outcome while in reality the treatment is just a mediator of that effect (as it happens to be on the causal pathway between the confounder and the outcome).
Random assignment eliminates the influence of the confounding variables on the treatment since it distributes them at random between the study groups, therefore, ruling out this alternative path or explanation of the outcome.

3. Random assignment also eliminates other threats to internal validity
By distributing all threats (known and unknown) at random between study groups, participants in both the treatment and the control group become equally subject to the effect of any threat to validity. Therefore, comparing the outcome between the 2 groups will bypass the effect of these threats and will only reflect the effect of the treatment on the outcome.
These threats include:
- History: This is any event that co-occurs with the treatment and can affect the outcome.
- Maturation: This is the effect of time on the study participants (e.g. participants becoming wiser, hungrier, or more stressed with time) which might influence the outcome.
- Regression to the mean: This happens when the participants’ outcome score is exceptionally good on a pre-treatment measurement, so the post-treatment measurement scores will naturally regress toward the mean — in simple terms, regression happens since an exceptional performance is hard to maintain. This effect can bias the study since it represents an alternative explanation of the outcome.
Note that randomization does not prevent these effects from happening, it just allows us to control them by reducing their risk of being associated with the treatment.
What if random assignment produced unequal groups?
Question: What should you do if after randomly assigning participants, it turned out that the 2 groups still differ in participants’ characteristics? More precisely, what if randomization accidentally did not balance risk factors that can be alternative explanations between the 2 groups? (For example, if one group includes more male participants, or sicker, or older people than the other group).
Short answer: This is perfectly normal, since randomization only assures an unbiased assignment of participants to groups, i.e. it produces comparable groups, but it does not guarantee the equality of these groups.
A more complete answer: Randomization will not and cannot create 2 equal groups regarding each and every characteristic. This is because when dealing with randomization there is still an element of luck. If you want 2 perfectly equal groups, you better match them manually as is done in a matched pairs design (for more information see my article on matched pairs design ).
This is similar to throwing a die: If you throw it 10 times, the chance of getting a specific outcome will not be 1/6. But it will approach 1/6 if you repeat the experiment a very large number of times and calculate the average number of times the specific outcome turned up.
So randomization will not produce perfectly equal groups for each specific study, especially if the study has a small sample size. But do not forget that scientific evidence is a long and continuous process, and the groups will tend to be equal in the long run when a meta-analysis aggregates the results of a large number of randomized studies.
So for each individual study, differences between the treatment and control group will exist and will influence the study results. This means that the results of a randomized trial will sometimes be wrong, and this is absolutely okay.
BOTTOM LINE:
Although the results of a particular randomized study are unbiased, they will still be affected by a sampling error due to chance. But the real benefit of random assignment will be when data is aggregated in a meta-analysis.
Limitations of random assignment
Randomized designs can suffer from:
1. Ethical issues:
Randomization is ethical only if the researcher has no evidence that one treatment is superior to the other.
Also, it would be unethical to randomly assign participants to harmful exposures such as smoking or dangerous chemicals.
2. Low external validity:
With random assignment, external validity (i.e. the generalizability of the study results) is compromised because the results of a study that uses random assignment represent what would happen under “ideal” experimental conditions, which is in general very different from what happens at the population level.
In the real world, people who take the treatment might be very different from those who don’t – so the assignment of participants is not a random event, but rather under the influence of all sort of external factors.
External validity can be also jeopardized in cases where not all participants are eligible or willing to accept the terms of the study.
3. Higher cost of implementation:
An experimental design with random assignment is typically more expensive than observational studies where the investigator’s role is just to observe events without intervening.
Experimental designs also typically take a lot of time to implement, and therefore are less practical when a quick answer is needed.
4. Impracticality when answering non-causal questions:
A randomized trial is our best bet when the question is to find the causal effect of a treatment or a risk factor.
Sometimes however, the researcher is just interested in predicting the probability of an event or a disease given some risk factors. In this case, the causal relationship between these variables is not important, making observational designs more suitable for such problems.
5. Impracticality when studying the effect of variables that cannot be manipulated:
The usual objective of studying the effects of risk factors is to propose recommendations that involve changing the level of exposure to these factors.
However, some risk factors cannot be manipulated, and so it does not make any sense to study them in a randomized trial. For example it would be impossible to randomly assign participants to age categories, gender, or genetic factors.
6. Difficulty to control participants:
These difficulties include:
- Participants refusing to receive the assigned treatment.
- Participants not adhering to recommendations.
- Differential loss to follow-up between those who receive the treatment and those who don’t.
All of these issues might occur in a randomized trial, but might not affect an observational study.
- Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference . 2nd edition. Cengage Learning; 2001.
- Friedman LM, Furberg CD, DeMets DL, Reboussin DM, Granger CB. Fundamentals of Clinical Trials . 5th ed. 2015 edition. Springer; 2015.
Further reading
- Posttest-Only Control Group Design
- Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
- Randomized Block Design
No internet connection.
All search filters on the page have been cleared., your search has been saved..
- All content
- Dictionaries
- Encyclopedias
- Expert Insights
- Foundations
- How-to Guides
- Journal Articles
- Little Blue Books
- Little Green Books
- Project Planner
- Tools Directory
- Sign in to my profile No Name
- Sign in Signed in
- My profile No Name
Reader's guide
Entries a-z, subject index.
- Random Assignment of Participants
- By: Amy May
- In: The SAGE Encyclopedia of Communication Research Methods
- Chapter DOI: https:// doi. org/10.4135/9781483381411
- Subject: Communication and Media Studies , Sociology
- Show page numbers Hide page numbers
Bias is an inherent part of the research process, and scholars must proactively address sources of bias to ensure validity. While bias has the potential to impact each stage of the research process, how participants are assigned to experimental groups is an important part of research design, as selection bias may be introduced. With selection bias, certain members of a sample population are intentionally omitted or targeted based on a specific trait(s). To reduce selection bias, random assignment of participants is used.
With random assignment, participants have an equal chance of being assigned to an experimental or control group, resulting in a sample that is, in theory, representative of the population. Random assignment helps ensure comparable groups, minimizing the influence of individual characteristics, ...
- Random Assignment
Random Effects Analysis
- Authoring: Telling a Research Story
- Body Image and Eating Disorders
- Hypothesis Formulation
- Methodology, Selection of
- Program Assessment
- Research Ideas, Sources of
- Research Project, Planning of
- Research Question Formulation
- Research Topic, Definition of
- Research, Inspiration for
- Social Media: Blogs, Microblogs, and Twitter
- Testability
- Acknowledging the Contribution of Others
- Activism and Social Justice
- Anonymous Source of Data
- Authorship Bias
- Authorship Credit
- Confidentiality and Anonymity of Participants
- Conflict of Interest in Research
- Controversial Experiments
- Copyright Issues in Research
- Cultural Sensitivity in Research
- Data Security
- Debriefing of Participants
- Deception in Research
- Ethical Issues, International Research
- Ethics Codes and Guidelines
- Fraudulent and Misleading Data
- Funding Research
- Health Care Disparities
- Human Subjects, Treatment of
- Informed Consent
- Institutional Review Board
- Organizational Ethics
- Peer Review
- Plagiarism, Self-
- Privacy of Information
- Privacy of Participants
- Public Behavior, Recording of
- Reliability, Unitizing
- Research Ethics and Social Values
- Researcher-Participant Relationships
- Social Implications of Research
- Archive Searching for Research
- Bibliographic Research
- Databases, Academic
- Foundation and Government Research Collections
- Library Research
- Literature Review, The
- Literature Reviews, Foundational
- Literature Reviews, Resources for
- Literature Reviews, Strategies for
- Literature Sources, Skeptical and Critical Stance Toward
- Literature, Determining Quality of
- Literature, Determining Relevance of
- Meta-Analysis
- Publications, Scholarly
- Search Engines for Literature Search
- Vote Counting Literature Review Methods
- Abstract or Executive Summary
- Academic Journals
- Alternative Conference Presentation Formats
- American Psychological Association (APA) Style
- Archiving Data
- Blogs and Research
- Chicago Style
- Citations to Research
- Evidence-Based Policy Making
- Invited Publication
- Limitations of Research
- Modern Language Association (MLA) Style
- Narrative Literature Review
- New Media Analysis
- News Media, Writing for
- Panel Presentations and Discussion
- Pay to Review and/or Publish
- Peer Reviewed Publication
- Poster Presentation of Research
- Primary Data Analysis
- Publication Style Guides
- Publication, Politics of
- Publications, Open-Access
- Publishing a Book
- Publishing a Journal Article
- Research Report, Organization of
- Research Reports, Objective
- Research Reports, Subjective
- Scholarship of Teaching and Learning
- Secondary Data
- Submission of Research to a Convention
- Submission of Research to a Journal
- Title of Manuscript, Selection of
- Visual Images as Data Within Qualitative Research
- Writer’s Block
- Writing a Discussion Section
- Writing a Literature Review
- Writing a Methods Section
- Writing a Results Section
- Writing Process, The
- Coding of Data
- Content Analysis, Definition of
- Content Analysis, Process of
- Content Analysis: Advantages and Disadvantages
- Conversation Analysis
- Critical Analysis
- Discourse Analysis
- Interaction Analysis, Quantitative
- Intercoder Reliability
- Intercoder Reliability Coefficients, Comparison of
- Intercoder Reliability Standards: Reproducibility
- Intercoder Reliability Standards: Stability
- Intercoder Reliability Techniques: Cohen’s Kappa
- Intercoder Reliability Techniques: Fleiss System
- Intercoder Reliability Techniques: Holsti Method
- Intercoder Reliability Techniques: Krippendorf Alpha
- Intercoder Reliability Techniques: Percent Agreement
- Intercoder Reliability Techniques: Scott’s Pi
- Metrics for Analysis, Selection of
- Narrative Analysis
- Observational Research Methods
- Observational Research, Advantages and Disadvantages
- Observer Reliability
- Rhetorical and Dramatism Analysis
- Unobtrusive Analysis
- Association of Internet Researchers (AoIR)
- Computer-Mediated Communication (CMC)
- Internet as Cultural Context
- Internet Research and Ethical Decision Making
- Internet Research, Privacy of Participants
- Online and Offline Data, Comparison of
- Online Communities
- Online Data, Collection and Interpretation of
- Online Data, Documentation of
- Online Data, Hacking of
- Online Interviews
- Online Social Worlds
- Social Networks, Online
- Correspondence Analysis
- Cutoff Scores
- Data Cleaning
- Data Reduction
- Data Trimming
- Facial Affect Coding System
- Factor Analysis
- Factor Analysis: Confirmatory
- Factor Analysis: Evolutionary
- Factor Analysis: Exploratory
- Factor Analysis: Internal Consistency
- Factor Analysis: Parallelism Test
- Factor Analysis: Rotated Matrix
- Factor Analysis: Varimax Rotation
- Factor Analysis-Oblique Rotation
- Implicit Measures
- Measurement Levels
- Measurement Levels, Interval
- Measurement Levels, Nominal/Categorical
- Measurement Levels, Ordinal
- Measurement Levels, Ratio
- Observational Measurement: Face Features
- Observational Measurement: Proxemics and Touch
- Observational Measurement: Vocal Qualities
- Organizational Identification
- Outlier Analysis
- Physiological Measurement
- Physiological Measurement: Blood Pressure
- Physiological Measurement: Genital Blood Volume
- Physiological Measurement: Heart Rate
- Physiological Measurement: Pupillary Response
- Physiological Measurement: Skin Conductance
- Reaction Time
- Reliability of Measurement
- Reliability, Cronbach’s Alpha
- Reliability, Knuder-Richardson
- Reliability, Split-half
- Scales, Forced Choice
- Scales, Likert Statement
- Scales, Open-Ended
- Scales, Rank Order
- Scales, Semantic Differential
- Scales, True/False
- Scaling, Guttman
- Standard Score
- Time Series Notation
- Validity, Concurrent
- Validity, Construct
- Validity, Face and Content
- Validity, Halo Effect
- Validity, Measurement of
- Validity, Predictive
- Variables, Conceptualization
- Variables, Operationalization
- Z Transformation
- Confederates
- Generalization
- Imagined Interactions
- Interviewees
- Matched Groups
- Matched Individuals
- Respondents
- Response Style
- Treatment Groups
- Vulnerable Groups
- Experience Sampling Method
- Sample Versus Population
- Sampling Decisions
- Sampling Frames
- Sampling, Internet
- Sampling, Methodological Issues in
- Sampling, Multistage
- Sampling, Nonprobability
- Sampling, Probability
- Sampling, Special Population
- Opinion Polling
- Sampling, Random
- Survey Instructions
- Survey Questions, Writing and Phrasing of
- Survey Response Rates
- Survey Wording
- Survey: Contrast Questions
- Survey: Demographic Questions
- Survey: Dichotomous Questions
- Survey: Filter Questions
- Survey: Follow-up Questions
- Survey: Leading Questions
- Survey: Multiple-Choice Questions
- Survey: Negative-Wording Questions
- Survey: Open-Ended Questions
- Survey: Questionnaire
- Survey: Sampling Issues
- Survey: Structural Questions
- Surveys, Advantages and Disadvantages of
- Surveys, Using Others’
- Under-represented Group
- Alternative News Media
- Analytic Induction
- Archival Analysis
- Artifact Selection
- Autoethnography
- Axial Coding
- Burkean Analysis
- Close Reading
- Coding, Fixed
- Coding, Flexible
- Computer-Assisted Qualitative Data Analysis Software (CAQDAS)
- Covert Observation
- Critical Ethnography
- Critical Incident Method
- Critical Race Theory
- Cultural Studies and Communication
- Demand Characteristics
- Ethnographic Interview
- Ethnography
- Ethnomethodology
- Fantasy Theme Analysis
- Feminist Analysis
- Field Notes
- First Wave Feminism
- Fisher Narrative Paradigm
- Focus Groups
- Frame Analysis
- Garfinkling
- Gender-Specific Language
- Grounded Theory
- Hermeneutics
- Historical Analysis
- Informant Interview
- Interaction Analysis, Qualitative
- Interpretative Research
- Interviews for Data Gathering
- Interviews, Recording and Transcribing
- Marxist Analysis
- Meta-ethnography
- Metaphor Analysis
- Narrative Interviewing
- Naturalistic Observation
- Negative Case Analysis
- Neo-Aristotelian Method
- New Media and Participant Observation
- Participant Observer
- Pentadic Analysis
- Performance Research
- Phenomenological Traditions
- Poetic Analysis
- Postcolonial Analysis
- Power in Language
- Pronomial Use-Solidarity
- Psychoanalytic Approaches to Rhetoric
- Public Memory
- Qualitative Data
- Queer Methods
- Queer Theory
- Researcher-Participant Relationships in Observational Research
- Respondent Interviews
- Rhetoric as Epistemic
- Rhetoric, Aristotle’s: Ethos
- Rhetoric, Aristotle’s: Logos
- Rhetoric, Aristotle’s: Pathos
- Rhetoric, Isocrates’
- Rhetorical Artifact
- Rhetorical Method
- Rhetorical Theory
- Second Wave Feminism
- Snowball Subject Recruitment
- Social Constructionism
- Social Network Analysis
- Spontaneous Decision Making
- Symbolic Interactionism
- Terministic Screens
- Textual Analysis
- Thematic Analysis
- Theoretical Traditions
- Third-Wave Feminism
- Transcription Systems
- Triangulation
- Turning Point Analysis
- Unobtrusive Measurement
- Visual Materials, Analysis of
- t -Test, Independent Samples
- t -Test, One Sample
- t -Test, Paired Samples
- Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA)
- Analysis of Ranks
- Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)
- Bonferroni Correction
- Decomposing Sums of Squares
- Eta Squared
- Factorial Analysis of Variance
- McNemar Test
- One-Tailed Test
- One-Way Analysis of Variance
- Post Hoc Tests
- Post Hoc Tests: Duncan Multiple Range Test
- Post Hoc Tests: Least Significant Difference
- Post Hoc Tests: Scheffe Test
- Post Hoc Tests: Student-Newman-Keuls Test
- Post Hoc Tests: Tukey Honestly Significance Difference Test
- Repeated Measures
- Between-Subjects Design
- Blocking Variable
- Control Groups
- Counterbalancing
- Cross-Sectional Design
- Degrees of Freedom
- Delayed Measurement
- Ex Post Facto Designs
- Experimental Manipulation
- Experiments and Experimental Design
- External Validity
- Extraneous Variables, Control of
- Factor, Crossed
- Factor, Fixed
- Factor, Nested
- Factor, Random
- Factorial Designs
- False Negative
- False Positive
- Field Experiments
- Hierarchical Model
- Individual Difference
- Internal Validity
- Laboratory Experiments
- Latin Square Design
- Longitudinal Design
- Manipulation Check
- Measures of Variability
- Median Split of Sample
- Mixed Level Design
- Multitrial Design
- Null Hypothesis
- One-Group Pretest–Posttest Design
- Orthogonality
- Overidentified Model
- Pilot Study
- Population/Sample
- Power Curves
- Quantitative Research, Purpose of
- Quantitative Research, Steps for
- Quasi-Experimental Design
- Replication
- Research Proposal
- Sampling Theory
- Sampling, Determining Size
- Solomon Four-Group Design
- Stimulus Pre-test
- Two-Group Pretest–Posttest Design
- Two-Group Random Assignment Pretest–Posttest Design
- Variables, Control
- Variables, Dependent
- Variables, Independent
- Variables, Latent
- Variables, Marker
- Variables, Mediating Types
- Variables, Moderating Types
- Within-Subjects Design
- Analysis of Residuals
- Bivariate Statistics
- Bootstrapping
- Confidence Interval
- Conjoint Analysis
- Contrast Analysis
- Correlation, Pearson
- Correlation, Point-Biserial
- Correlation, Spearman
- Covariance/Variance Matrix
- Cramér’s V
- Discriminant Analysis
- Kendall’s Tau
- Kruskal-Wallis Test
- Linear Regression
- Linear Versus Nonlinear Relationships
- Multicollinearity
- Multiple Regression
- Multiple Regression: Block Analysis
- Multiple Regression: Covariates in Multiple Regression
- Multiple Regression: Multiple R
- Multiple Regression: Standardized Regression Coefficient
- Partial Correlation
- Phi Coefficient
- Semi-Partial r
- Simple Bivariate Correlation
- Categorization
- Cluster Analysis
- Data Transformation
- Errors of Measurement
- Errors of Measurement: Attenuation
- Errors of Measurement: Ceiling and Floor Effects
- Errors of Measurement: Dichotomization of a Continuous Variable
- Errors of Measurement: Range Restriction
- Errors of Measurement: Regression Toward the Mean
- Frequency Distributions
- Heterogeneity of Variance
- Heteroskedasticity
- Homogeneity of Variance
- Hypothesis Testing, Logic of
- Intraclass Correlation
- Mean, Arithmetic
- Mean, Geometric
- Mean, Harmonic
- Measures of Central Tendency
- Mortality in Sample
- Normal Curve Distribution
- Relationships Between Variables
- Sensitivity Analysis
- Significance Test
- Simple Descriptive Statistics
- Standard Deviation and Variance
- Standard Error
- Standard Error, Mean
- Statistical Power Analysis
- Type I error
- Type II error
- Univariate Statistics
- Variables, Categorical
- Variables, Continuous
- Variables, Defining
- Variables, Interaction of
- Autoregressive, Integrative, Moving Average (ARIMA) Models
- Binomial Effect Size Display
- Cloze Procedure
- Cross Validation
- Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis
- Curvilinear Relationship
- Effect Sizes
- Hierarchical Linear Modeling
- Lag Sequential Analysis
- Logistic Analysis
- Log-Linear Analysis
- Margin of Error
- Markov Analysis
- Maximum Likelihood Estimation
- Meta-Analysis: Estimation of Average Effect
- Meta-Analysis: Fixed Effects Analysis
- Meta-Analysis: Literature Search Issues
- Meta-Analysis: Model Testing
- Meta-Analysis: Random Effects Analysis
- Meta-Analysis: Statistical Conversion to Common Metric
- Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA)
- Multivariate Statistics
- Ordinary Least Squares
- Path Analysis
- Probit Analysis
- Structural Equation Modeling
- Time-Series Analysis
- Acculturation
- African American Communication and Culture
- Agenda Setting
- Applied Communication
- Argumentation Theory
- Asian/Pacific American Communication Studies
- Bad News, Communication of
- Basic Course in Communication
- Business Communication
- Communication and Aging Research
- Communication and Culture
- Communication and Evolution
- Communication and Future Studies
- Communication and Human Biology
- Communication and Technology
- Communication Apprehension
- Communication Assessment
- Communication Competence
- Communication Education
- Communication Ethics
- Communication History
- Communication Privacy Management Theory
- Communication Skills
- Communication Theory
- Conflict, Mediation, and Negotiation
- Corporate Communication
- Crisis Communication
- Cross-Cultural Communication
- Cyberchondria
- Dark Side of Communication
- Debate and Forensics
- Development of Communication in Children
- Digital Media and Race
- Digital Natives
- Dime Dating
- Disability and Communication
- Distance Learning
- Educational Technology
- Emergency Communication
- Empathic Listening
- English as a Second Language
- Environmental Communication
- Family Communication
- Feminist Communication Studies
- Film Studies
- Financial Communication
- Freedom of Expression
- Game Studies
- Gender and Communication
- GLBT Communication Studies
- GLBT Social Media
- Group Communication
- Health Communication
- Health Literacy
- Human-Computer Interaction
- Instructional Communication
- Intercultural Communication
- Intergenerational Communication
- Intergroup Communication
- International Communication
- International Film
- Interpersonal Communication
- Intrapersonal Communication
- Language and Social Interaction
- Latino Communication
- Legal Communication
- Managerial Communication
- Mass Communication
- Massive Multiplayer Online Games
- Massive Open Online Courses
- Media and Technology Studies
- Media Diffusion
- Media Effects Research
- Media Literacy
- Message Production
- Multiplatform Journalism
- Native American or Indigenous Peoples Communication
- Nonverbal Communication
- Organizational Communication
- Parasocial Communication
- Patient-Centered Communication
- Peace Studies
- Performance Studies
- Personal Relationship Studies
- Philosophy of Communication
- Political Communication
- Political Debates
- Political Economy of Media
- Popular Communication
- Pornography and Research
- Public Address
- Public Relations
- Reality Television
- Relational Dialectics Theory
- Religious Communication
- Rhetorical Genre
- Risk Communication
- Robotic Communication
- Science Communication
- Selective Exposure
- Service Learning
- Small Group Communication
- Social Cognition
- Social Network Systems
- Social Presence
- Social Relationships
- Spirituality and Communication
- Sports Communication
- Strategic Communication
- Structuration Theory
- Training and Development in Organizations
- Video Games
- Visual Communication Studies
- Wartime Communication
- Academic Journal Structure
- Citation Analyses
- Communication Journals
- Interdisciplinary Journals
- Professional Communication Organizations (NCA, ICA, Central, etc.)
Sign in to access this content
Get a 30 day free trial, more like this, sage recommends.
We found other relevant content for you on other SAGE platforms.
Have you created a personal profile? Login or create a profile so that you can save clips, playlists and searches
- Sign in/register
Navigating away from this page will delete your results
Please save your results to "My Self-Assessments" in your profile before navigating away from this page.
Sign in to my profile
Sign up for a free trial and experience all SAGE Research Methods has to offer.
You must have a valid academic email address to sign up.
Get off-campus access
- View or download all content my institution has access to.
- view my profile
Statistical Thinking: A Simulation Approach to Modeling Uncertainty
Internal validity evidence and random assignment.
Medical researchers may be interested in showing that a drug helps improve people’s health (the cause of improvement is the drug), while educational researchers may be interested in showing a curricular innovation improves students’ learning (the curricular innovation causes improved learning). To attribute a causal relationship, there are three criteria a researcher needs to establish:
- Temporal Precedence: The cause needs to happen BEFORE the effect.
- Covariation of the Cause and Effect: There needs to be a correlational relationship between the cause and effect.
- No Plausible Alternative Explanations: ALL other possible explanations for the effect need to be ruled out.
Because of this third criteria, attributing a cause-and-effect relationship is very difficult. (You can read more about each of these criteria at the Web Center for Social Research Methods .)
Experimental studies have their strength in meeting this third criteria. To rule out ALL other possible explanations for the effect, the control group and the treatment group need to be “identical” with respect to every possible characteristic (aside from the treatment) that could explain differences. This way the only characteristic that will be different is that the treatment group gets the treatment and the control group doesn’t. If there are differences in the outcome, then it must be attributable to the treatment, because the other possible explanations are ruled out.
So, the key is to make the control and treatment groups “identical” when you are forming them. One thing that makes this task (slightly) easier is that they don’t have to be exactly identical, only probabilistically equivalent . This means, for example, that if you were matching groups on age that you don’t need the two groups to have identical age distributions; they would only need to have roughly the same AVERAGE age. Here roughly means “the average ages should be the same within what we expect because of sampling error.”
Now we just need to create the groups so that they have, on average, the same characteristics … for EVERY POSSIBLE CHARCTERISTIC that could explain differences in the outcome. Zoinks! 13
It turns out that creating probabilistically equivalent groups is a really difficult problem. One method that works pretty well for doing this is to randomly assign participants to the groups. This works best when you have large sample sizes, but even with small sample sizes random assignment has the advantage of at least removing the systematic bias between the two groups (any differences are due to chance and will probably even out between the groups). As Wikipedia’s page on random assignment points out,
Random assignment of participants helps to ensure that any differences between and within the groups are not systematic at the outset of the experiment. Thus, any differences between groups recorded at the end of the experiment can be more confidently attributed to the experimental procedures or treatment. … Random assignment does not guarantee that the groups are matched or equivalent. The groups may still differ on some preexisting attribute due to chance. The use of random assignment cannot eliminate this possibility, but it greatly reduces it.
Internal validity is the degree to which cause-and-effect inferences are accurate and meaningful. Causal attribution is the goal for many researchers. Thus, by using random assignment we have a pretty high degree of evidence for internal validity; we have a much higher belief in causal inferences. Much like evidence used in a court of law, it is useful to think about validity evidence on a continuum. We will visualize this continuum as a barometer. For example, a barometer visualizing the internal validity evidence for a study that employed random assignment in the design might be:

The degree of internal validity evidence is high (in the upper-third). How high depends on other factors such as sample size.
To learn more about random assignment, you can read the following:
- The research report, Random Assignment Evaluation Studies: A Guide for Out-of-School Time Program Practitioners
According to Wiktionary the earliest usage of the work “zoinks” was by Norville “Shaggy” Rogers on the show Scooby-Doo. ↩︎

IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Random assignment refers to the use of chance procedures in psychology experiments to ensure that each participant has the same opportunity
Random assignment is a procedure used in experiments to create multiple study groups that include participants with similar characteristics so that the groups
In an experiment, random assignment means placing your participants into control and experimental groups at random.
Random assignment is a fundamental part of a “true” experiment because it helps ensure that any differences found between the groups are attributable to the
Random assignment or random placement is an experimental technique for assigning human participants or animal subjects to different groups in an experiment
Random assignment is most related to design. In fact, when we randomly assign participants to treatments we have, by definition, an experimental design.
In an experimental study, random assignment is a process by which participants are assigned, with the same chance, to either a treatment or
Random assignment prevents this self sorting of participants and reduces the likelihood that the groups start with systematic differences. In fact, studies have
With random assignment, participants have an equal chance of being assigned to an experimental or control group, resulting in a sample that is, in theory
Random assignment of participants helps to ensure that any differences between and within the groups are not systematic at the outset of the experiment. Thus
Even for a sample of 200. Page 14. HOW OFTEN RANDOM ASSIGNMENT FAILS. 14 participants, exceeding the average sample size in social psychology's top journals in